For other versions of this document, see http://wikileaks.org/wiki/CRS-RL31825 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ ˘ ˇ ˘ ˘ Prepared for Members and Committees of Congress ˇ ˘ ˘ The purpose of Personal Reemployment Accounts (PRAs) is to provide persons likely to exhaust their Unemployment Insurance (UI) benefits a choice in the type and source of reemployment services and to induce UI claimants and exhaustees to speed their reemployment by providing a bonus equal to the balance in their Personal Reemployment Accounts when they obtain new jobs. PRA recipients would have to pay from their accounts for some reemployment services now available free at one-stop centers, if they chose to utilize those services (e.g., training). The Bush Administration initially proposed the accounts in its 2003 economic stimulus package and included a PRA demonstration in its FY2005 budget request. In March 2005, the House passed H.R. 27, a bill that would reauthorize the Workforce Investment Act (WIA) and authorize a PRA demonstration. (During the 108th Congress, the House passed H.R. 444, which included PRA provisions identical to those in H.R. 27.) The U.S. Department of Labor announced in October 2004 that seven states had been selected to participate in a PRA PY2004 demonstration. Funding for the demonstration was not included in FY2005 appropriations, and the Administration did not request PRA funds for FY2006. In September 2005, it proposed recovery accounts for workers displaced from jobs as a result of the Gulf Coast hurricanes that appear to be much like PRAs. H.R. 3976, introduced in October 2005, similarly would create worker recovery accounts for those who lost jobs due to Hurricanes Katrina and Rita. Proponents of the concept point to the results of experiments conducted during the 1980s as support for the efficacy of a reemployment bonus. The experiments produced mixed results: only one of six bonus designs in Washington significantly reduced the length of unemployment and the amount of UI benefits, while all but one of the designs in Pennsylvania significantly sped reemployment; in both states, the costs of the bonus experiments exceeded their benefits to the UI system. Based upon data from the two experiments, it was subsequently estimated that targeting bonus offers to likely UI exhaustees yielded somewhat better outcomes than untargeted bonuses and that varying the value of the bonus could affect the number of weeks UI benefits must be paid as well as the bonus receipt rate. Another PRA simulation involving likely UI exhaustees in Georgia estimated that 59% would not have qualified for a bonus; it suggested that PRA recipients might reduce their use of fee-based reemployment services to maximize the bonus value. The design of the experiments differs from the PRA demonstrations in H.R. 27 and the PY2004 project in ways that could affect the applicability of the experiments' results. For example, those eligible for the demonstrations would be more disadvantaged than experiment participants. The experiments' bonus amount generally was a fixed amount, while the demonstration participants could affect its value based on their use of fee-based services. Further, experiment participants had to remain employed for a given time before qualifying for a bonus; demonstration participants would receive a portion of their PRA balances immediately upon reemployment. This report will be updated as warranted. ˇ ˘ Personal Reemployment Accounts and the Workforce Investment Act .......................................... 1 A Comparison of the Design of Bonus Experiments and of the Current Bonus Proposal .............. 3 Eligible Participants .................................................................................................................. 3 Bonus Amount........................................................................................................................... 4 Qualification Period .................................................................................................................. 5 Reemployment Period............................................................................................................... 5 Results of Selected State Bonus Experiments ................................................................................. 6 The Washington Reemployment Bonus Experiment ................................................................ 6 The Pennsylvania Reemployment Bonus Experiment .............................................................. 8 Reanalysis of the Washington and Pennsylvania Experiments ............................................... 10 A Targeted Bonus.............................................................................................................. 10 What Happens When You Change Program Characteristics?........................................... 10 Illinois Claimant Bonus Experiment ........................................................................................11 Table 1. Treatments in the Washington Experiment ........................................................................ 6 Table 2. Treatments in the Pennsylvania Experiment...................................................................... 9 Author Contact Information .......................................................................................................... 12 ˇ ˘ A s part of its 2003 economic stimulus package, the Bush Administration proposed Personal Reemployment Accounts (PRAs). The Secretary of Labor testified in February 2003 before the House Committee on Education and the Workforce that the accounts are intended not only to speed reemployment into stable jobs through provision of a cash bonus to Unemployment Insurance (UI) claimants that states determine are likely to exhaust their benefits, but also to give the targeted individuals an opportunity to choose the type of services (e.g., retraining, child care, and transportation) and the source of services (i.e., one-stop delivery system or other service providers) they believe will be most useful to them in their pursuit of new jobs. Proponents of the accounts point to the results of experiments conducted in four states during the mid- to late 1980s as support for the efficacy of offering a new-jobs bonus to UI claimants. In her testimony, the Secretary stated that experiments in Washington, Pennsylvania, and New Jersey showed that a reemployment bonus of $300 to $1,000 motivated the recipients to become reemployed, reduced the duration of UI by almost one week, and resulted in new jobs comparable in earnings to those obtained by workers who were not eligible for the bonus and remained unemployed longer.1 Further, she noted that in an experiment that took place in Illinois, "a reemployment bonus of $500 reduced the duration of unemployment by more than a week and did not lead to lower earnings at the worker's next job." This report begins by briefly summarizing the relevant provisions contained in legislation pending before the 109th Congress. It then compares the design elements of the state bonus experiments with those of the bonus demonstrations included in legislation to reauthorize the Workforce Investment Act and in a Program Year (PY) 2004 project of the U.S. Department of Labor (DOL). The report closes with a review of the results of selected state bonus experiments. ˘ On January, 4, 2005, two bills were introduced in the House that would authorize a PRA demonstration under the Workforce Investment Act (WIA): H.R. 26, a stand-alone bill, and H.R. 27, a broader bill to reauthorize WIA. (WIA, 29 U.S.C. §2811 et seq.).2 Both bills contain identical language about the demonstration project under Section 171 of WIA to test and evaluate PRAs.3 On March 2, 2005, the House passed H.R. 27. The principle features of the PRA demonstration are as follows: 1 U.S. Congress, House Committee on Education and the Workforce, 108th Congress, 1st sess., Back to Work: The Administration's Plan for Economic Recovery and the Workforce Investment Act, Feb. 12, 2003, Serial no. 108-1, pp. 59-60 (statement of Elaine Chao, Secretary of Labor). 2 For more information on WIA, see CRS Report 97-536, Job Training Under the Workforce Investment Act: An Overview, by Ann Lordeman. 3 The PRA provisions of H.R. 26 and H.R. 27 are identical to the provisions contained in H.R. 444, passed by the House in the 108th Congress. ˇ ˘ · States and other eligible entities would provide accounts of up to $3,000 per eligible individual. The initial account balances must be the same for participants within a local area or within a state. · Eligible individuals primarily would be UI claimants identified through worker profiling as likely to exhaust their benefits and in need of job search assistance to obtain new employment.4 Eligible entities could, at their option, extend accounts to individuals who had exhausted their benefit entitlement and (a) are in training for which completion requires additional support or (b) were laid off from an industry or occupation with declining employment or which no longer provides jobs in a local area. · Individuals could use the account funds, at their own discretion, to purchase a variety of employment-related services (i.e., intensive services, training, and support services and assistance with buying or leasing a car) on a fee-for-service basis from the one-stop delivery system or other service providers. Recipients could only receive training, intensive, and support services on a fee-for-service basis during the one-year period from the date of establishment of their accounts. Core services provided by one-stop centers would remain free to account recipients (e.g., access to job listings and assistance with writing résumés). · Individuals with PRAs who obtain full-time jobs within a 13-week qualification period (dating from initial receipt of UI benefits for those still eligible for benefits and from the account's establishment for those who had exhausted their benefits) would receive any funds remaining in their accounts as bonuses. The bonuses would be dispensed in two installments: 60% upon reemployment and 40% six months thereafter. These features are similar to an $8 million PRA demonstration for which the U.S. Department of Labor (DOL) announced awards to seven states on October 29, 2004.5 For FY20005, the President had requested $50 million under the authority of WIA Section 171 for a PRA demonstration. Funding for the demonstration was not included in FY2005 appropriations (P.L. 108-447). The Administration did not request PRA funds for FY2006. As part of the Administration's response to Hurricanes Katrina and Rita, it proposed recovery accounts--seemingly like PRAs--for workers displaced from their jobs as a result of the natural disaster. H.R. 3976, introduced in October 2005, similarly would create worker recovery accounts for workers who lost jobs due to the Gulf Coast hurricanes and who are likely to exhaust the benefits they are receiving under the Disaster Unemployment Assistance or Unemployment Insurance programs. No action has been taken on this approach to aiding workers displaced as a result of the hurricanes. 4 P.L. 103-152, which amended the Social Security Act, required states to develop systems of profiling new UI claimants in order to identify those likely to exhaust their benefits. After states screen out initial claimants on recall status (including in some cases persons laid off from seasonal industries) and those who exclusively use union hiring halls, the states use statistical models or characteristic screens to identify potential UI exhaustees. Measures taken into account include previous occupation, industry, wages, and job tenure; educational attainment and other claimant characteristics; local economic indicators; and the individual's UI weekly benefit amount. States are not permitted to include such equal opportunity characteristics as age, race, gender, and disability status. 5 The seven states are Florida, Idaho, Minnesota, Mississippi, Montana, Texas and West Virginia. For more information, see http://www.dol.gov/opa/media/press/opa/OPA20042248.htm. ˇ ˘ ˇ Bonus experiments were conducted in Washington, Pennsylvania, New Jersey, and Illinois. This report omits the New Jersey experiment because it had a very limited eligible population (i.e., dislocated workers, who were defined as persons having worked for the same employer for three years before filing for UI benefits). As a result, the findings from the experiment are not generalizeable to a bonus program having a broader eligible population such as the one currently proposed. New Jersey's bonus experiment had a mandatory job-search component as well.6 The experiments in Washington, Pennsylvania, and Illinois had a number of elements in common. Differences between the design of the experiments and the current bonus proposal will affect how well the results from the former can be applied to the latter. In the experiments, UI claimants were randomly assigned to a control (i.e., comparison) group that received existing services and to one or more treatment groups that received, in addition to existing services, time-limited offers of bonuses which they were awarded after a specified period of reemployment. Accordingly, treatment group members did not differ from typical UI recipients in any particular way (e.g., they were not more likely to exhaust their benefits). In addition, participants in the experiments excluded persons who already had exhausted their UI benefits. On both counts, then, individuals in the bonus experiments were likely to have better reemployment prospects than the eligible population of current legislation and DOL's PY2004 demonstration. Even among persons profiled as most likely to exhaust UI benefits, a PRA simulation utilizing employment service records of Georgia UI claimants found that 59% would have been ineligible for a bonus given their reemployment histories. About two-fifths found jobs within 13 weeks and consequently would have qualified for an initial (60%) bonus payment. A little more than one- fourth remained employed long enough to have received both bonus payments.7 6 The experiment also does not provide "much guidance for policy, because ... bonus offers were made only after seven weeks of insured unemployment, and the pending offer was unknown to the selected participants prior to that time. Such a situation could not be replicated in a real program, as knowledge of the pending offer would be available to all claimants from the start of their benefit year (and probably prior to that ... ). This knowledge can be expected to critically affect job-search behavior during the first seven weeks of the benefit year, as well as during the period in which the bonus was available." Walter A. Corson and Robert G. Spiegelman, "Introduction and Background of the Reemployment Bonus Experiments," in Philip K. Robins and Robert G. Spiegelman, eds., Reemployment Bonuses in the Unemployment Insurance System (Kalamazoo, MI: W.E. Upjohn Institute for Employment Research, 2001), p. 14. (Hereafter cited as Robins and Spiegelman, Reemployment Bonuses in the Unemployment Insurance System.) 7 Christopher J. O'Leary and Randall Eberts, "Personal Reemployment Accounts," Employment Research, vol. 11, no. 1 (Jan. 2004). (Hereafter cited as O'Leary and Eberts, Personal Reemployment Accounts.) See also Christopher J. O'Leary and Randall Eberts, Personal Reemployment Accounts: Simulations for Planning Implementation, W.E. Upjohn Institute Staff Working Paper No. 04-110, May 2004. ˇ ˘ In most treatments, the value of the bonus offer was a multiple of an individual claimant's weekly benefit amount (WBA). The dollar value of bonuses therefore differed from one claimant to another because of the claimants' varying benefit entitlements. The bonus offers were the same, however, in terms of the opportunity cost of unemployment to claimants (i.e., a reduction in one or more weeks of unemployment cost each person one or more weeks of UI benefits). As described in current proposals and the PY2004 demonstration, there is no link between the value of a bonus and the value of an individual's UI benefit. Instead, variability within a state in participants' bonuses (i.e., in their PRA balances) would depend upon their differing utilization of fee-based reemployment services. While persons in the experiments could not control the size of their bonuses, individuals in the proposed program could do so through their decisions about expenditures from the accounts. Some might try to maximize their potential bonus by relying only on the free services that still would be available to them at one-stop centers. Those in areas with relatively low unemployment rates or with signs of an improving economy could be more likely to think they need few, if any, fee-based reemployment services. Some information on the possible utilization of different reemployment services can be gleaned from the PRA simulation of likely UI exhaustees in Georgia. The study found that relatively few persons drew upon training and supportive services during their first 13 weeks of benefit receipt. In contrast, about two-fifths utilized two intensive services, namely, customer service plans and counseling. The researchers estimated that the intensive services cost $356 and $712, respectively--much less than the little-used options of training and supportive services ($1,424 and $1,068, respectively). The implementation of a PRA program might affect this usage, however, as participants would be faced with having to pay for some services. The analysts further estimated that if individuals spent the entire PRA amount of $3,000 to buy services because they believed doing so would speed reemployment or help get higher paying jobs, the unemployed workers "must expect either earnings to be nearly 14 percent higher or that employment will occur at least 6 weeks sooner." As they noted that earnings and employment gains of this magnitude are not supported by prior research, "PRA recipients might therefore reduce use of services in hopes of receiving larger reemployment bonuses."8 Another difference between the experiments and the currently proposed design with regard to the value of the bonus is the amounts available to treatment group members were below the $3,000 maximum value. In 1989 dollars, the year when the experiments mostly took place, $3,000 was equivalent to $2,040--considerably more than the $300-$1,000 range of bonuses in the experiments.9 Of course, eligible entities would be free under the PRA provisions in H.R. 27 and in the PY2004 demonstration to set a bonus amount below $3,000, which would, in turn, allow them to make offers to a larger share of eligible UI recipients. However, while $3,000 might appear to be a generous bonus amount, it might not seem so in terms of covering the cost of reemployment services for which account funds could be used. The adequacy of the PRA amount would vary by 8 O'Leary and Eberts, Personal Reemployment Accounts, pp. 3-4. 9 Paul T. Decker and Irma L. Perez-Johnson, What Can We Expect Under Personal Reemployment Accounts? Predictions and Procedures, Mathematica Policy Research, Inc., Jan. 23, 2004. (Hereafter cited as Decker and Perez- Johnson, What Can We Expect Under Personal Reemployment Accounts?.) ˇ ˘ geographic area as well, in terms of both its component uses. For example, the value of the bonus would be greater to someone living in an area with a lower cost of living, and reemployment services presumably would be less costly to someone residing in an area with a community college.10 Treatment group members who found new full-time jobs within a given period could apply for a bonus.11 It was thought that the optimal "qualification period" should not be so long that many UI claimants could obtain bonuses without altering their behavior (i.e., receive bonuses without increasing their job search effort in order to become reemployed sooner than they otherwise would have). Alternatively, it was thought that the qualification period should not be so brief that it discouraged claimants from attempting to intensify their job-seeking activities. Treatment group members generally received the same bonus amount (e.g., six times their WBA) regardless of when within the qualification period they obtained new jobs. It was estimated that the one experimental treatment in Pennsylvania that provided a declining bonus amount over the qualification period did not significantly hasten an individual's reemployment. As currently proposed, bonus amounts would decline over the 13-week qualification period if individuals draw upon their PRAs. Individuals effectively would control the rate at which their bonuses decreased. It thus appears that, under the currently proposed design, participants could themselves determine whether they faced a constant or a declining bonus offer. ˘ UI recipients in the experiments had to remain employed for a prescribed length of time before receiving bonuses. Its length was chosen to avoid providing a bonus for unstable seasonal work and to reduce an individual's inclination to take any job just to qualify for a bonus. As currently proposed, individuals would get 60% of their bonus amounts immediately upon reemployment. Those in the state experiments got none of their bonuses until completion of the reemployment period. The reemployment period was about four months in each experiment, which is two months shorter than the reemployment period required in H.R. 27 and the PY2004 demonstration to obtain the remaining 40% of the bonus. Immediate receipt of a large share of a substantial bonus might be a greater inducement to speedy reemployment and acceptance of a low-paying job compared to the experiments' designs. The longer reemployment period for receipt of the remaining portion might be comparatively less of an inducement for remaining in the new job, particularly if the value of the bonus were fairly small. In other words, the actual amount of the bonus could influence whether the timing of its receipt makes any difference to the individual's job search and retention behavior. The researchers who conducted the PRA simulation of Georgia UI beneficiaries noted that, while "the reemployment earnings of those 10 Ibid. 11 Jobless persons on recall status generally could not obtain bonuses if they subsequently got back their previous jobs at the employers who laid them off. This also typically was the case for jobless persons who obtained employment through union hiring halls because, in both instances, the bonus offer did not cause them to change their behavior. Instead of the bonus offer prompting them to accelerate their job search in order to obtain employment sooner than they otherwise would have, they continued to rely on the actions of others (i.e., former employers and referral unions) to determine the timing of their reemployment. ˇ ˘ offered bonuses [in the experiments] were at least a high as the control groups, [t]he timing of bonus payments under the proposed PRAs might yield a different impact on wages."12 ˇ The goals of the experiments were two-fold. One was to reduce the amount of unemployment, and the other was to reduce the cost of the states' Unemployment Trust Funds. The eligible population for participation in the experiments therefore was restricted to UI beneficiaries. The key outcome measures were the difference between the treatment and control groups in the duration of insured unemployment and in the amount of UI benefits received. ˘ ˇ UI claimants were randomly assigned to one of six treatment groups or to a control group. The treatments involved variations on the size of the bonus offered and the length of the qualification period. Although the bonuses shown in Table 1 were calculated as a multiple of an individual's UI weekly benefit amount (WBA), the actual offers made to persons assigned to the treatment groups were expressed as dollar amounts. Qualification period aside, the lowest bonus amount averaged about $300; the middle bonus amount, about $600; and the highest bonus amount, about $900. The reemployment period was set at four months. The evaluators determined that only the highest bonus level of six times a claimant's WBA produced a large and statistically significant effect on job search behavior by reducing both the length of unemployment and the amount of benefits. In 2003, the average WBA in Washington was about $324. The highest bonus level in current dollars thus would be equal to $1,944 on average, which is almost two-thirds of the proposed $3,000 maximum for PRAs that are meant to go toward both reemployment services and bonuses. The highest bonus level and the long qualification period had the largest impact, namely, a 0.75- week reduction in unemployment and a $140 reduction in UI benefits. The average response across all treatment groups was a 0.41-week reduction in the duration of UI payments and a $65 reduction in benefits. The most cost-effective of the approaches also was estimated to be Treatment 6 (i.e., shifting from lower bonus offers and the short qualification period to the maximum bonus and qualification period had a statistically significant impact on both outcome measures). 1 elbaT tnemirepxE notgnihsaW eht ni stnemtaerT. noitacifilauQ adoirep tnuoma sunoB trohS b gnoL c )ABW( tnuoma tifeneb ylkeew s'tnamialc a semit owT 1 puorg tnemtaerT 4 puorg tnemtaerT 12 O'Leary and Eberts, Personal Reemployment Accounts, p. 4. 13 This section is derived from U.S. Department of Labor, The Washington Reemployment Bonus Experiment Final Report, Unemployment Insurance Occasional Paper 92-6, 1992. ˇ ˘ doirep noitacifilauQ a tnuoma sunoB cgnoL btrohS 5 puorg tnemtaerT 2 puorg tnemtaerT ABW s'tnamialc a semit ruoF 6 puorg tnemtaerT 3 puorg tnemtaerT ABW s'tnamialc a semit xiS a ,tnemeltitne tifeneb IU fo noitarud rieht htiw detaicossa era stroffe hcraes-boj 'slaudividni taht gnimussA .a staert )skeew fo rebmun deificeps a ot desoppo sa( noitarud fo erahs deificeps a si taht doirep noitacifilauq ni tnemeltitne tifeneb 'stnamialC .tnemeltitne tifeneb fo shtgnel gniyrav htiw etats a ni emas eht stnamialc lla .skeew 03 ot skeew 01 morf degnar notgnihsaW noitacifilauq trohs ehT .doirep gnitiaw keew-eno eht sulp tnemeltitne tifeneb s'tnamialc a fo tnecrep ytnewT .b .skeew xis tsomla saw egareva eht ;skeew neves ot skeew eerht morf degnar doirep noitacifilauq gnol ehT .doirep gnitiaw keew-eno eht sulp tnemeltitne tifeneb s'tnamialc a fo tnecrep ytroF .c .skeew 11 saw egareva eht ;skeew 31 ot skeew evif morf degnar doirep Individuals assigned to the treatment groups were found to have exited the unemployment rolls sooner than control group members during the qualification period. The quicker reemployment of treatment group members was not associated with their accepting poorer quality jobs as measured by lower hourly wages or fewer hours worked. There were limited differences in the job search responses of subgroups within the treatment groups. For example, older workers (i.e., age 45 or older) with high earnings (i.e., in the top third of the earnings distribution or at least $17,366) experienced among the largest effects in terms of shortened unemployment and reduced UI benefits. The evaluators speculated that this might have been the case because older, high-earning workers could have become discouraged from looking for work after they had been let go from jobs they had held for many years. These individuals thus had room to improve upon their job search efforts. For all dislocated workers, however, only those with the broadest definition used by the evaluators (i.e., individuals continuously employed for three years prior to filing for UI) and who were offered the largest bonus had outcome measures that differed substantially from others in the treatment groups. Similarly, the effects of the experiment on unemployment duration and UI payments differed according to economic conditions. The evaluators found stronger responses to the bonus offer in areas within the state with low unemployment rates (i.e., below 5%). Workers in those areas might have thought that increasing their job search efforts could well elicit more job offers; workers in areas with higher unemployment rates might have thought that employer demand was too soft to generate additional opportunities regardless of their job search intensity. This finding suggests that a bonus offer could have limited impact during a cyclical downturn of the economy or in an area experiencing long-term decline. The evaluators looked at whether the experiment conferred net benefits or costs from the perspective of the UI system, all government, society as a whole, and selected population groups. It was assumed that in an actual bonus program the bonuses would come from the UI Trust Fund; therefore, the cost to the UI system was considered to be bonus payments and administrative expenses while the benefit was considered to be savings in UI payments due to earlier reemployment. The UI system was estimated to experience net costs because the reduction in benefits was smaller than the bonuses paid.14 In other words, the experiment did not pay for itself. 14 The administrative expenses of the experiment were quite low at $3 per eligible claimant. ˇ ˘ If a regular bonus program were to be implemented, this finding suggests that a source other than the UI Trust Fund would have to be found to fund it (e.g., WIA). The experiment was slightly more beneficial to all government than to the UI system because benefits, in addition to reduced UI payments, included the income tax on earnings that reemployed workers owed the government. From a societal perspective, the reemployment bonus was estimated to produce large net benefits because the increase in earnings of those who responded to the bonus offer far outweighed the small increase in administrative costs associated with a bonus program.15 The net benefits to society estimated for the experiment could be overstated if the bonus receipt rate were higher in an established program. (Up to one-third of the individuals in the experiment who were entitled to bonuses did not collect them.16) In terms of a bonus offer to subgroups within the population of UI recipients, the evaluators found that a program directed at older high-earners had large net benefits for the UI system, government, and society. ˘ ˘ ˇ UI claimants were randomly assigned to one of six treatment groups or to a control group. The treatments involved variations on the size of the bonus offered and the duration of the qualification period, as shown in Table 2.18 As in the Washington experiment, the bonus offers made to persons assigned to the treatment groups were expressed as a dollar amount although the bonuses were calculated as a multiple of each claimant's WBA. Qualification period aside, the lower bonus amount averaged $500; the higher, $1,000. The reemployment period was set at 16 weeks. Unlike in Washington, all treatments in Pennsylvania included the option of attending a job search workshop. Treatment 6 did not include access to the workshop in order to isolate its impact. However, participation in the workshop was so low (2.6%) that it prevented estimation of its impact and resulted in there actually being five rather than six approaches. The evaluators speculated that participants lacked interest in attending the workshop because of the state's low unemployment rate and to the workshop being offered early in an individual's jobless spell. 15 Transfer payments (i.e., UI benefits and bonus amounts) cancel each other out when computing costs and benefits to society as a whole because they represent benefits to some (i.e., UI recipients) and costs to others (i.e., taxpayers). 16 The net benefits of the experiment also could differ from the long-run results of an ongoing program for reasons other than an increased take-up rate. For example, persons offered bonuses to speed their reemployment might take jobs that other jobseekers would have taken. Because the former's reemployment would come at the expense of longer unemployment among the latter, the net benefits of a bonus program would be lower. In addition, because a bonus offer could increase the value of filing a claim for UI benefits, it might prompt more UI-eligible workers to register with the system and thereby increase the number of persons receiving UI benefits and potentially receiving bonus payments. 17 Unless otherwise indicated, this section is derived from U.S. Department of Labor, The Pennsylvania Reemployment Bonus Experiment Final Report, Unemployment Insurance Occasional Paper 92-1, 1992. 18 Unlike Washington's variable lengths of UI benefit entitlement, virtually all claimants in Pennsylvania were entitled to 26 weeks of payments. Thus, fixed qualification periods of six weeks and 12 weeks achieved the homogenous treatment of persons offered bonuses. (For more information see Walter A. Corson and Robert G. Spiegelman, "Design of Three Field Experiments," in Robins and Spiegelman, Reemployment Bonuses in the Unemployment Insurance System.) ˇ ˘ Also unlike in Washington, Pennsylvania included a treatment (5) with a declining bonus amount over the qualification period. It was hypothesized that structuring the bonus in this way would encourage claimants to become reemployed as early in the longer qualification period as possible. tnemirepxE ainavlysnneP eht ni stnemtaerT.2 elbaT doirep noitacifilauQ tnuoma sunoB skeew 21 skeew 6 2 puorg tnemtaerT 1 puorg tnemtaerT )ABW( tnuomA tifeneB ylkeeW s'tnamialc a semit eerhT 4 puorg tnemtaerT 3 puorg tnemtaerT ABW s'tnamialc a semit xiS a 5 puorg tnemtaerT -- gninilced neht dna ABW s'tnamialc a semit xis yllaitinI b6 puorg tnemtaerT -- pohskrow hcraes-boj lanoitpo on dna ABW s'tnamialc a semit xiS fo reffo eht edulcni ton did rettal eht taht saw 6 dna 4 spuorg tnemtaerT neewteb ecnereffid ylno ehT .a .dednetta tnemirepxe eht ni stnapicitrap wef yrev hcihw ,pohskrow hcraes-boj a gnidnetta .pohskrow hcraes-boj a gnidnetta fo noitpo eht dereffo erew xis tpecxe spuorg tnemtaert llA .b The evaluators determined that all of the treatments--with the exception of the declining bonus offer--significantly reduced both the duration of unemployment and the amount of UI benefits. The highest bonus level and the long qualification period (Treatment 4) had the largest impact, namely, a 0.8-week reduction in unemployment and a $130 reduction in UI benefits. These are almost the same magnitudes produced by a very similar approach, Treatment 6, in the Washington experiment. (The other Pennsylvania treatments yielded a reduction in the duration of UI payments of 0.5 weeks on average and a reduction in benefits of $80 on average.) Pennsylvania's Treatment 4 also was the only design that significantly lowered the share of claimants who exhausted their UI benefits. In 2003, the average WBA in Pennsylvania was about $292. Treatment 4, at six times a claimant's WBA, would produce a bonus in current dollars equal to $1,752 on average, which is almost 60% of the proposed $3,000 maximum for PRAs that are intended to be used for both reemployment services and bonuses. Individuals assigned to the treatment groups were found to have exited the unemployment rolls sooner than control group members during the qualification period. As the differential in UI exit rates tended to be widest right before or at the end of the qualification period, many treatment group members appear to have escalated their job search activities just before their bonus eligibility expired. There was no evidence that the comparatively faster reemployment of treatment group members meant they accepted less desirable jobs in order to get a bonus. Very little difference was found in the job search response to a bonus offer among subgroups within the treatment groups. The impact of the treatments on UI receipt was substantial and significantly greater among claimants from manufacturing industries than from other industries (by more than 1.5 weeks), however. The evaluators looked at whether the experiment conferred net benefits or costs from a variety of perspectives. The findings from the Pennsylvania experiment were similar to those from the Washington experiment. Despite the bonus offer producing a significant decrease in UI receipt, the experiment was not cost-effective because the reduction in benefits did not outweigh the cost to the UI system of paying bonuses. The outcome for the government was somewhat better, with ˇ ˘ some treatments producing net benefits as a result of the income taxes on increased earnings from workers who qualified for the bonus. Society as a whole was estimated to experience net benefits because the workers' increased earnings exceeded the program's low administrative costs. ˘ ˘ ˇ Researchers employed data from the Washington and Pennsylvania experiments to estimate the effects of a reemployment bonus focused on UI recipients with high likelihoods of benefit exhaustion. Models of the probability of benefit exhaustion were developed for the two states. They were similar to the Worker Profiling and Reemployment Services (WPRS) systems that states use to identify UI claimants expected to experience long spells of unemployment. The researchers examined the effects of limiting bonus offers to individuals with varying probabilities of benefit exhaustion. They found that very narrowly focusing bonuses on those most likely to exhaust benefits (e.g., claimants in the top 10% of the exhaustion probability distribution) was not necessarily the most effective approach. Neither the Washington nor Pennsylvania experiments produced greater reductions in weeks of UI receipt when they were targeted at the top 50% rather than the top 25% of persons most likely to exhaust their benefits. The savings in UI benefits were larger for the top 25% or the top 50% of potential exhaustees than for all bonus experiment participants. Conversely, bonus and administrative costs were higher in the targeted design than in the non-targeted designs. From the perspective of the UI system, net benefits of the combined treatments were greater (or net costs smaller) for the targeted design than for the non-targeted designs.20 Among the various bonus treatments in Washington and Pennsylvania, the researchers estimated that a low bonus amount with a long qualification period (e.g., three times the WBA and a 12-week qualification period) that is focused on the 50% of UI claimants most likely to exhaust benefits was the most cost-effective treatment. They concluded that while "the original findings from the [non-targeted] experiments did not generate overwhelming support for reemployment bonuses," some degree of targeting based on worker profiling "improves the appeal of the reemployment bonus program."21 Another study also utilized data from the Washington and Pennsylvania experiments to, among other things, estimate the bonus receipt rate and UI impact if the PRA amount is changed. The researchers found that if UI recipients were offered a $3,000 bonus, 33% would qualify for and 19 This section was derived from Christopher J. O'Leary, Paul T. Decker, and Stephen A. Wandner, "Cost- Effectiveness of Targeted Reemployment Bonuses," Journal of Human Resources, winter 2005, vol. 40, no. 1. (Hereafter cited as O'Leary, Decker, and Wandner, Cost-Effectiveness of Targeted Reemployment Bonuses.) 20 Defined as UI benefit savings plus UI tax contributions on additional earnings minus bonus payment and program administration costs. 21 O'Leary, Decker, and Wandner, Cost-Effectiveness of Targeted Reemployment Bonuses, p. 278. 22 This section was derived from Decker and Perez-Johnson, What Can We Expect Under Personal Reemployment Accounts?. ˇ ˘ receive the first bonus installment; if UI recipients targeted for WPRS were offered a $3,000 bonus, 31% would qualify for and receive the first installment equal to 60% of the account's balance. This exceeds the bonus receipt rates of the state experiments not only because of the PRA's relatively greater value, but also because reemployment under H.R. 27 triggers immediate payment of 60% of the account's balance compared to the experiments' requirement that individuals retain their jobs for 16 weeks before they receive bonuses. In contrast, a lower PRA amount should yield a lower receipt rate because it reduces people's incentive to try for the bonus. Thus, a $2,000 bonus could reduce the receipt rate of the first installment to 27% for UI recipients generally and 30% for those targeted for WPRS. If the bonus were lowered to $1,000-- about the amount (in real terms) offered in the state experiments--the receipt rates (20% and 29%, respectively) could still be above the actual rates in the experiments. Similarly, the researchers estimated that the effect on the length of UI receipt would somewhat exceed the impacts in the state experiments. The latter ranged between -0.26 weeks to -0.82 weeks per person offered bonuses with a qualification period of 11 or 12 weeks. Alternatively, a $3,000 bonus was found to reduce the length of UI receipt by 1.66 weeks per account recipient; $2,000, by -1.38 weeks; and $1,000, by -1.09 weeks. The reason for the potentially superior performance of the PRA vis-a-vis the state experiments is twofold: the PRA amount would be higher, which should accelerate reemployment and thereby reduce the benefit period; and the PRA would be focused on those expected to have long durations of UI receipt, which should increase the average contraction in benefit weeks for persons who find jobs during the bonus qualification period. ˇ In this earliest experiment, new UI claimants were randomly assigned to one treatment and one control group. The treatment involved the offer of a $500 bonus for reemployment full-time within 11 weeks of filing for benefits and after having been in the job for four months. The bonus amount was equal to about four weeks of UI payments. The 11-week qualification period represented about 40% of the UI potential benefit entitlement in Illinois' regular program (i.e., 26 weeks). The $500 bonus was estimated to have produced a large and statistically significant effect on job search behavior by reducing the average length of unemployment by 1.15 weeks. The bonus also lowered UI benefit receipt significantly: the average benefit of those in the treatment group was $158 or $194 less than received by those in the control, depending on whether Federal Supplemental Compensation (FSC) benefits were included. The FSC program ended during the Illinois experiment, which meant about half the claimants had 26 weeks of UI eligibility under the state's regular program and about half had 38 weeks because of the supplemental program. It was determined that the two groups had very different responses to the bonus offer. The 1.15-week reduction in UI benefit receipt was the product of a 1.78-week reduction among FSC-eligibles and a 0.71-week reduction among FSC-ineligibles. The response of the latter group more closely resembles the results from the Washington and Pennsylvania experiments, which suggests that the Illinois results overstate the reduction in unemployment 23 Unless otherwise indicated, this section was derived from Stephen A. Woodbury and Robert G. Spiegelman, "Bonuses to Workers and Employers to Reduce Unemployment: Randomized Trials in Illinois," American Economic Review, vol. 77, no. 4 (1987). ˇ ˘ duration that could be expected from a bonus focused on claimants in states' regular UI programs.24 Relatively more individuals assigned to the treatment group than to the control group exited the UI rolls during the qualification period and relatively fewer treatment than control group members exhausted their benefits. As in the Pennsylvania and Washington experiments, the comparatively faster reemployment of treatment group members in Illinois was not accompanied by lower earnings. It was estimated that the experiment conferred large net benefits from the perspective of the UI system. This outcome greatly differs from those of the Washington and Pennsylvania experiments. The discrepancy was due to the much larger reduction in weeks of UI receipt found in Illinois. As noted above, however, when Illinois' treatment group is made more comparable to the other two states by limiting its claimants to those in the regular UI program, the decrease in duration of UI receipt more nearly matches the experience in Washington and Pennsylvania. Linda Levine Ann Lordeman Specialist in Labor Economics Specialist in Social Policy llevine@crs.loc.gov, 7-7756 alordeman@crs.loc.gov, 7-2323 24 Paul T. Decker, Christopher J. O'Leary, and Stephen A. Woodbury, "Bonus Impacts on Receipt of Unemployment Insurance," in Robins and Spiegelman, Reemployment Bonuses in the Unemployment Insurance System. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ For other versions of this document, see http://wikileaks.org/wiki/CRS-RL31825